Comment
Broxmeyer's blog is useful in that it describes the work of Fischer, whose contribution to the understanding of AD is rarely mentioned, and his suggestion that a new type of bacterium might be added to the list of microbes possibly involved in AD is interesting but perhaps no great surprise. However, his contention that HSV1 is not involved in AD (he maintains that it has been “struck down”, along with Chlamydia pneumoniae, and several types of spirochetes) seems to be based solely on a single assumption in a review by Mawanda and Wallace [1]. Unfortunately for all three authors though, the assumption is totally fallacious: it states that if the frequency of infection with a microbe in brain is similar in AD patients and normal subjects (as we and others have shown is the case with HSV1), that microbe cannot be a factor in the disease. Broxmeyer, and Mawanda and Wallace, need to recall that many people can be infected by a microbe yet are asymptomatic, and so are classed as “controls” (i.e., they appear to be uninfected). The most striking example of this is the TB bacterium—Mycobacter tuberculosis, which infects approximately one-third of the world's population yet only some 10% actually develop TB. Similarly with HSV1, the usual cause of cold sores, only about 25% of the many who are infected with the virus display any sores, despite the frequency of reactivation of the virus. Surprisingly on this theme, Mawanda and Wallace state elsewhere in the review, referring to a study by Deatly et al. [2]: “Notably, in situ hybridization of postmortem brain tissue samples from 21 patients with AD and 19 controls detected HSV-1 DNA in a significantly higher proportion of AD samples (81%) than controls (47.4%)”, but Mawanda and Wallace do not even intimate if this supports or undermines their case. However, Deatly et al. found these percentages not for the brain but for the trigeminal ganglion, and what was sought was “latent HSV1 RNA” because of its presumed much higher level, not HSV1 DNA! In fact, Deatly et al. found “no evidence of viral RNA in the central nervous system”.
What is notable is that Deatly et al. [2] stated that the sensitivity of the in situ hybridisation method they used for detecting HSV1 RNA was one viral sequence per cell, which is vastly lower than that of DNA detectable by PCR (e.g., the sensitivity detected by my group in 1997 [3] was about one viral sequence per 104 cells). As for detection of serum antibodies to HSV1 which, anyway, in early publications, showed little consistency, two of the three older references cited by Mawanda and Wallace were published 27 and 30 years ago when, as with DNA, the sensitivity of antibody detection would have been much lower; their third reference, 125, cites instead my lab’s study (and definite detection) of intrathecal antibodies to HSV1! However, referring elsewhere to recent studies, they state that “Serologic analysis .... has also linked HSV-1 to increased AD risk”. Mawanda and Wallace do acknowledge in the case of DNA, though, that usage of PCR has increased the detection sensitivity of specific sequences. Nonetheless, their case, and hence that of Broxmeyer, is fatally undermined by these fallacies and factual errors. Real errors and fallacies need to be struck down, not phantom ones; tilting at windmills has never been a wise pursuit.
References
[1] Mawanda F, Wallace R (2013) Can infections cause Alzheimer's disease? Epidemiol Rev 35, 161-180.
[2] Deatly MA, Haase AT, Fewsters PH, Lewis E, Ball MJ (1990) Human herpes virus infections and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropathol Appl Neurohiol 16, 213-223.
[3] Itzhaki RF, Lin W-R, Shang D, Wilcock GK, Faragher B, Jamieson GA (1997) Herpes simplex virus type 1 in brain and risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet 349, 241-244.
- Comment
|